To see and not to see
Ver e enxergar
Uma experiêcia interessante sobre ver e enxergar
O que acontece quando um adulto que foi cego desde a infância e subitamente recupera sua visão?
A experiência de
Virgil, um homem de 50 anos, que recuperou sua visão após 45 anos
cego levanta questionamentos sobre percepção que tem perseguido
filósofos e cientistas por séculos.
Esta historia deu origem a um relato feito por Oliver Sacks, neurologista, que
foi usada para fazer um filme feito por Mira Sorvino e Val Kilmer, "À
Primeira Vista", ou "To
see and no to see".
O filme em si, como historia, é cheio de clichês e não foi
bem aceito pela crítica, embora seja razoável de se assistir.
O que é memorável é o trabalho de Val Kilmer, que visitou
a pessoa que foi tratada por Oliver Sacks e conseguiu transmitir com perfeição
a situação de uma pessoa que não teve o aprendizado de
ver e saber o que está vendo.
É o complemento perfeito para o caso
dos africanos relatado por McLuhan.
Por exemplo, ele, que está acostumado com a espacialidade que o cerca
ser vista com o tato e a audição, quando passa a enxergar, não
consegue decodificar aquilo em termos de se posicionar fisicamente e realizar
tarefas que fazemos automaticamente sem perceber.
No caso dos africanos, eles não são educados como nós para imaginar uma historia com começo, meio e fim, onde, para nós toda a realidade refletida numa tela contem elementos que assumimos pela nossa imaginação terem um posicionamento lógico, definido e inquestionável, que faz com que quase tudo que nós é mostrado, nós não enxergamos, mas vemos aquilo para o qual fomos treinados a fazer.
Isto é muito difícil de entender, e para isto, a sequência deste filme que vai dos 58 minutos, quando ele recupera a visão após uma cirurgia até a cena, à 1:08, neste filme, quando Virgil vendo (mas não sabendo do que se trata) está na rua, no meio dos automóveis e do tráfego, vai atravessar uma porta e entrar num edifício, mesmo vendo a porta de vidro e o movimento dos automóveis, não consegue relacionar com o que aquilo significa, ficando literalmente perdido, inclusive quase se machucando, até que fecha os olhos e se localiza pelo tato e pela audição, onde está e como está, como fazia quando era cego. Ele procura um terapeuta para aprender ver e enxergar.
Este filme, na sua estrutura, ambientação, contexto da relação, etc., é cliché, mas a sequência entre 58 minutos até mais ou menos 1:20, é absolutamente preciosa para entender o que McLuhan relatou no caso dos africanos, que somente viram a galinha naquele filme educacional. E, claro, pensando sobre os mecanismos que atuam no caso geral da humanidade educada a encontrar em livros o aprendizado sobre como entender o que se vê, isto é, enxergamos o que vemos porque nos educamos. No caso, o meio, isto é, a visão, é a mensagem.
Fiz
um estudo detalhado sobre o "meio" para
o caso de sons.
Converto para o caso da
visão, de forma sintética, para entendermos melhor o que está
em jogo e como tudo isto atua na nossa percepção e o que provoca
está condição para o caso de Virgil e para o caso dos africanos.
A historia de Oliver Sacks é a seguinte:
TO SEE AND NOT SEE
What happens
when an adult who has been blind since childhood suddenly has his vision restored?
The experience of Virgil, a fifty-year-old Oklahoman who regained his sight
after forty~five years, raises questions
about perception that have haunted philosophers and scientists for centuries.
BY OLIVER SACKS
The New Yorker, May 10, 1993
EARLY in October of 1991,
1 got a phone call from a retired minister in the Midwest, who told me about
his daughter's fiance, a
fifty-year old man named Virgil, who had been virtually blind since early childhood.
He had thick cataracts, and was also said to have
retinitis pigmentosa, a hereditary condition that slowly but implacably eats
away at the retinas. But his fianc6e, Amy, who required
regular eye checks herself, because of diabetes, had recently taken him to see
her own ophthalmologist, Dr. Scott Hamlin, and he had
given them new hope. Dr. Hamlin, listening carefully to the history, was not
so sure that Virgil had retinitis pigmentosa. It was difficult
to be certain at this stage, because the retinas could no longer be seen beneath
the thick cataracts, but Virgil could still see light and
dark, the direction from which light came, and the shadow of a hand moving in
front of his eyes, so obviously there was not a total
destruction of the retina. And cataract extraction was a relatively simple procedure,
done under local anesthesia, with very little
surgical risk. There was nothing to lose - and there might be much to gain.
Amy and Virgil would be getting married soon - wouldn't it
be fantastic if he could see? If, after nearly a lifetime of blindness, his
first vision could be his bride, the wedding, the minister, the
church!
Dr. Hamlin had agreed to operate, and the cataract on Virgils right eye
had been removed a fortnight earlier, Amys father
informed me. And, miraculously, the operation had worked. Amy, who began keeping
a journal the day after the operation-the day the
bandages were removed-wrote in her initial entry: Virgil can SEE!... Entire
office in tears, first time Virgil has sight for forty years....
Virgil's family so excited, crying, can't believe it! ... Miracle of sight restored
incredible!" But the following day she remarked
problems: "Trying to adjust to being sighted, tough to go from blindness
to sighted. Has to think faster, not able to trust vision yet....
Like baby just learning to see, everything new, exciting, scary, unsure of what
seeing means."
A neurologists life is not systematic, like a scientists, but it
provides him with novel and unexpected situations, which can
become windows, peepholes, into the intricacy of nature - an intricacy that
one might not anticipate from the ordinary course of life.
"Nature is nowhere accustomed more openly to display her secret mysteries,"
wrote Thomas Willis, in the seventeenth century, "than
in cases where she shows traces other workings apart from the beaten path."
Certainly this phone call-about the restoration of vision in
adulthood to a patient blind from early childhood-hinted of such a case. 'In
fact," writes the ophthalmologist Alberto Valvo, in "Sight
Restoration after Long-Term Blindness" (1971), "the number of cases
of this kind over the last ten centuries known to us is not more
than twenty."
What would vision be like in such a patient? Would it be "normal"
from the moment vision was restored? This is what one
might think at first. This is the commonsensical. Notion - that the eyes will
be opened, the scales will fall from them, and (in the words
of the New Testament) the blind man will receive" sight.
But could it be that simple? Was not experience necessary to see? Did one not
have to learn to see? I was not well acquainted
with the literature on the subject, though I had read with fascination the great
case history published in the Quarterly Journal of
Psycbology in 1963 by the psychologist Richard Gregory (with Jean G. Wallace),
and I knew that such cases, hypothetical or real, had
riveted the attention of philosophers an psychologists for hundreds of years.
(There is a hint of it even in the Bible, in Marks
description of the miracle at Bethsaida; for here, at first, the blind man saw
"men as trees, walking," and only subsequently was his
eyesight fully restored.) The seventeenth-century philosopher William Molyneux,
whose wife was blind, posed the following question
to his friend John Locke: "Suppose a man born blind, and now adult, and
taught by his touch to distinguish between a cube and a
sphere [be] made to see: [could he now] by his sight, before he touched them
... distinguish and tell which was the globe and which the
cube?" Locke considers this in his "Essay Concerning Human Understanding"
(1690) and decides that the answer is no. In 1709,
examining the problem in more detail, and the whole relation between sight and
touch, in "A New Theory of Vision," George Berkeley
concluded that there was no necessary connection between a tactile world and
a sight world-that a connection between them could be
established only on the basis of experience.
Barely twenty years elapsed before these considerations were put to the test-when,
in 1728, an English surgeon named
William Cheselden removed the cataracts from the eyes of a thirteen-year old
boy born blind. Despite his high intelligence and youth,
the boy encountered profound difficulties with the simplest visual perceptions.
He had no idea of distance. He had no idea of space or
size. And he was bizarrely confused by drawings and paintings, by the idea of
a two-dimensional representation of reality. As Berkeley
had anticipated, he was able to make sense of what he saw only gradually, and
insofar as he was able to connect visual experiences
with tactile ones. It had been similar with many other patients in the two hundred
and fifty years since Cheselden's operation: nearly all
had experienced the most profound, Lockean confusion and bewilderment.
And yet, I was informed, as soon as the bandages were removed from Virgil's
eye he saw his doctor and his fiancee, and
laughed. Doubtless he saw sometbing but what did he see? What did seeing
for this previously not-seeing man mean? What sort of
world had he been launched into?
VIRGlL (nearly all the names in this account have been changed, and some ident4ing
details have been disguised) was born on a small
farm in Kentucky soon after the outbreak of the Second World War. He seemed
normal enough as a baby, but (his mother thought) had
poor eyesight even as a toddler, sometimes bumped into things, seemed not to
see them. At the age of three, he became gravely ill with
a triple illness-a meningitis or meningoencephalitis (inflammation of the brain
and its membranes), polio, and cat-scratch fever. During
this acute illness, he had convulsions, became virtually blind, paralyzed in
the legs, partly paralyzed in his breathing, and, after ten
days, fell into a coma. He remained in a coma for two weeks. When he emerged
from it, he seemed, according to his mother, "a
different person" and "sort of dull inside"; he showed a curious
indolence, nonchalance, passivity, seemed nothing at all like the
spunky, mischievous boy he had been.
The strength in his legs came back over the next year, and his chest grew stronger,
though never, perhaps, entirely normal. His
vision also recovered significantly - but his retinas were now gravely damaged.
Whether the retinal damage was caused wholly by his
acute illness or perhaps partly by a congenital retinal degeneration was never
dear.
In Virgils sixth year, cataracts began to develop in both eyes, and it
was evident that he was becoming functionally blind.
That same year, he was sent to a school for the blind, and there he eventually
learned to read Braille and to become adept with the use
of a cane. But he was not a star pupil; he was not as adventurous or aggressively
independent as some blind people are. There was a
striking passivity all through his time at school-as, indeed, there had been
since his illness.
Yet Virgil graduated from the school and, when he was twenty, decided to leave
Kentucky, to seek training, work, and a life
of his own in a city in Oklahoma. He trained as a massage therapist, and soon
found employment at a Y.M.C.A. He was obviously
good at his job, and highly esteemed, and the Y was happy to keep him on its
permanent staff and to provide a small house for him
across the road, where he lived with a friend, also employed at the Y. Virgil
had many clients-it is fascinating to hear the tactile detail
with which he can describe them-and seemed to take a real pleasure and pride
in his job. Thus, in his modest way, Virgil made a life:
had a steady job and an identity, was selfsupporting, had friends, read Braille
papers and books (though less, with the years, as Talking
Books came in). He had a passion for sports, especially baseball, and loved
to listen to games on the radio. He had an encyclopedic
knowledge of baseball games, players, scores, statistics. On a couple of occasions,
he became involved with girlfriends, and would
cross the city on public transport to meet them. He maintained a close tie with
home, and particularly with his mother - he would get
hampers of food regularly from the farm, and send hampers of laundry back and
forth. Life was limited, but stable in its way.
Then, in 1991, he met Amy - or, rather, they met again, for they had known each
other well twenty or more years before.
Amy's background was different from Virgil's: she came from a cultivated middle-class
family, had gone to college in New Hampshire,
and had a degree in botany. She had worked at another Y in town, as a swimming
coach, and had met Virgil at a cat show in 1968.
They dated a bit-she was in her early twenties, he was a few years older - but
then Amy decided to go back to graduate school in
Arkansas, where she met her first husband, and she and Virgil fell out of contact.
She ran her own plant nursery for a while,
specializing in orchids, but had to give this up when she developed severe asthma.
She and her first husband divorced after a few years,
and she returned to Oklahoma. In 1988, out of the blue, Virgil called her, and,
after three years of long phone calls between them, they
finally met again, in 1991. "All of a sudden it was like twenty years were
never there," Amy said.
Meeting again, at this point in their lives, both felt a certain desire for
companionship. With Amy, perhaps, this took a more
active form. She saw Virgil stuck (as she perceived it) in a vegetative, dull
life: going over to the Y, doing his massages; going back
home, where, increasingly, he listened to ballgames on the radio; going out
and meeting people less and less each year. Restoring his
sight, she must have felt, would, like marriage, stir him from his indolent
bachelor existence and provide them both with a new life.
But Virgil was passive here as in so much else. He had been sent to half a dozen
specialists over the years, and they had been
unanimous in declining to operate, feeling that in all probability he had no
useful retinal function; and Virgil seemed to accept this with
equanimity. But Amy disagreed. With Virgil being blind already, she said, there
was nothing to lose, and there was a real possibility,
remote but almost too exciting to contemplate, that he might actually get reasonable
sight back and, after forty-five years, see again.
And so Amy pushed for the surgery. Virgils mother, fearing disturbance,
was strongly against it. ("He is fine as he is," she said.)
Virgil himself showed no preference in the matter; he seemed happy to go along
with whatever they decided.
Finally, in mid-September, the day of surgery came. Virgils right eye
had its cataract removed, and a new lens implant was
inserted; then the eye was bandaged, as is customary, for twenty-four hours
of recovery. The following day, the bandage was removed,
and Virgil's eye was finally exposed, without cover, to the world. The moment
of truth had finally come.
Or had it? The truth of the matter (as I pieced it together later), if less
"miraculous" than Amys journal suggested, was
infinitely stranger. The dramatic moment stayed vacant, grew longer, sagged.
No cry ("I can see!") burst from Virgils lips. He seemed
to be staring blankly, bewildered, without focussing, at the surgeon, who stood
before him, still holding the bandages. Only when the
surgeon spoke - saying "Well?"- did a look of recognition cross Virgil's
face.
Virgil told me later that in this first moment he had no idea what he was seeing.
There was light, there was movement, there
was color, all mixed up, all meaningless, a blur. Then out of the blur came
a voice that said, 'Well?" Then, and only then, he said, did
he finally realize that this chaos of light and shadow was a face - and, indeed,
the face of his surgeon. His experience was virtually
identical to that of Gregory's patient, S.B.:
When the bandages were removed ... he heard a voice coming from in front of
him and to one side: he turned to the source of the sound, and saw
a "blur." He realized that this must be a face.... He seemed to think
that he would not have known that this was a face if he had not previously
heard the voice and known that voices came from faces.
The rest of us, born sighted, can scarcely imagine such confusion. For we, born
with a full complement of senses, and
correlating these, one with the other, create a sight world from the start,
a world of visual objects and concepts and meanings. When we
open our eyes each morning, it is upon a world we have spent a lifetime learning
to see. We are not given the world: we make our
world through incessant experience, categorization, memory, reconnection. But
when Virgil opened his eye, after being blind for
forty-five years-having had little more than an infant's visual experience,
and this long forgotten-there were no visual memories to
support a perception, there was no world of experience and meaning awaiting
him. He saw, but what he saw had no coherence. His
retina and optic nerve were active, transmitting impulses, but his brain could
make no sense of them; he was, as neurologists say,
agnosic.
Everyone, Virgil included, expected something much simpler. A man openshis eyes,
light enters, and falls on the retina: he
sees. It is as simple as that, we imagine. And the surgeon's own experience,
like that of most ophthalmologists, had been with the
removal of cataracts from patients who had almost always lost their sight late
in life - and such patients do indeed, if the surgery is
successful, have a virtually immediate recovery of normal vision, for they have
in no sense lost their ability to see. And so, though
there had been a careful surgical discussion of the operation and of possible
postsurgical complications, there was little discussion or
preparation for the neurological and psychological difficulties that Virgil
might encounter.
WITH the cataract out, Virgil was able to see colors and movements, to see (but
not identify) large objects and shapes, and,
astonishingly, to read some letters on the third line of the standard Snellen
eye chart-the line corresponding to a visual acuity of about
20/100, or a little better. But though his best vision was a respectable 20/80,
he lacked a coherent visual field, because his central
vision was poor, and it was almost impossible for the eye to fixate on targets;
it kept losing them, making random searching
movements, finding them, then losing them again. It was evident that the central,
or macular, part of the retina, which is specialized for
high acuity and fixation, was scarcely functioning, and that it was only the
surrounding paramacular area which was making possible
such vision as he had. The retina itself presented a moth-eaten or piebald appearance,
with areas of increased and decreased
pigmentation-islets of intact or relatively intact retina alternating with areas
of atrophy. The macula was degenerated and pale, and the
blood vessels of the entire retina appeared narrowed.
Examination, I was told, suggested the scars or residues of old disease but
no current or active disease process; and, this being
so, Virgil's vision, such as it was, could be stable for the rest of his life.
It could be hoped, moreover (since the worse eye had been
operated on first), that the left eye, which was to be operated upon in a few
weeks' time, might have considerably more functional
retina than the right.
I HAD not been able to go to Oklahoma straightaway~my impulse was to take the
next plane after that initial phone call-but had kept
myself informed of Virgil's progress over the ensuing weeks by speaking with
Amy, with Virgil's mother, and, of course, with Virgil
himself. I also spoke at length with Dr. Hamlin and with Richard Gregory, in
England-to discuss what sort of test materials I should
bring, for I myself had never seen such a case, nor did I know anyone (apart
from Gregory) who had. I gathered together some
materials--solid objects, pictures, cartoons, illusions, videotapes, and special
perceptual tests designed by a physiologist colleague,
Ralph Siegel; I phoned an ophthalmologist friend, Robert Wasserman (we had previously
worked together on the case of a color-blind
painter), and we started to Plan a visit. It was important, we felt, not just
to test Virgil but to see how he managed in real life, inside his
house, outside, in natural settings and social situations; crucial, too, that
we see him as a person, bringing his own life history~his
particular dispositions and needs and expectations-to this critical passage;
that we meet his fianc6e, who had so urged the operation,
and with whom his life was now so intimately mingled; that we look not merely
at his eyes and perceptual powers but at the whole
tenor and pattern of his life.
VIRGIL and Amy - now newlyweds - greeted us at the exit barrier in the airport.
He was of medium height, but exceedingly fat; he
moved slowly and tended to cough and puff with the slightest exertion. He was
not, it was evident, an entirely well man. His eyes roved
to and fro, in searching movements, and when Amy introduced Bob and me he did
not seem to see us straightaway - he looked toward
us but not quite at us. I had the impression, momentary but strong, that Virgil
did not really look at our faces, though he smiled and
laughed and listened minutely.
I was reminded of what Gregory had observed of his patient S.B.-that "he
did not find faces 'easy' objects," and that "he did
not look at a speaker's face, and made nothing of facial expressions."
Virgil's behavior was certainly not that of a sighted man, but it
wasn't that of a blind man, either. It was, rather, the behavior of one mentally
blind, or agnosicable to see but not to decipher what he
was seeing. He reminded me of an agnosic patient of mine, Dr. P. (the man who
mistook his wife for a hat), who, instead of looking at
me, taking me in, in the normal way, made sudden strange fixations - on my nose,
on my right ear, down to my chin, up to my right eye
- not seeing my face as a whole.
We walked out through the crowded airport, Amy holding VirgiFs arm, guiding
him, and out to the lot where they had parked
their car. Virgil was fond of cars, and one of his first pleasures after surgery
(as with S.B.) had been to watch them through the window
of his house, to enjoy their motions, and spot their colors and shapes - their
colors, especially. He was sometimes bewildered by
shapes. 'What cars do you see?" I asked him as we walked through the lot.
He pointed to all the cars we passed. "Thats a blue one,
thats red-wow, thats a big one!" Some of the shapes he found very surprising.
"Look at that one!" he exclaimed once. "I have to look
down!" And, bending, he felt it - it was a slinky, streamlined V-12 Jaguar
- and confinned its low profile. But it was only the colors and
general profiles he was getting; he would have walked past his own car had Amy
not been with him. And Bob and I were struck by the
fact that Virgil would look, would attend visually, only if one asked him to
or pointed something out - not spontaneously. His sight
might be largely restored, but seeing, looking, it was clear, was far from natural
to him; he still had many of the habits, the behaviors,
of a blind man.
The drive from the airport to their house was a long one; it took us through
the heart of town, and it gave us an opportunity to
talk to Virgil and Amy, and to observe Virgil's reactions to his new vision.
He clearly enjoyed movement, watching the ever-changing
spectacle through the car windows, and the movement of other cars on the road.
He spotted a speeder coming up very fast behind us,
and identified cars, buses (he especially loved the bright-yellow school buses),
eighteen-wheelers, and, once, on a side road, a slow,
noisy tractor. He seemed very sensitive to, and intrigued by, large neon signs
and advertisements, and liked picking out their letters as
we passed. He had difficulty reading entire words, though he often guessed them
correctly from one or two letters, or from the style of
the signs. Other signs he saw but could not read. He was able to see and identify
the changing colors of the traffic lights as we got into
town.
He and Amy told us of other things he had seen since his operation, and of some
of the unexpected confiisions that could
occur. He had seen the moon; it was larger than he expected. On one occasion,
he was puzzled by seeing "a fat airplane" in the sky -
"stuck, not moving." It turned out to be a blimp. Occasionally, he
had seen birds; they made him jump, sometimes, if they came too
close. (Of course, they did not come that close, Amy explained. Virgil simply
had no idea of distance.)
Much of their time recently had been spent shopping-there had been the wedding
to prepare for, and Amy wanted to show
Virgil off, tell his story to the clerks and shopkeepers they knew, let them
see a transformed Virgil for themselves. It was fun; the local
television station had aired a story about Virgils operation, and people
would recognize him and come up to shake his hand. But
supermarkets and other stores were also dense visual spectacles of objects of
all kinds, often in bright packaging, and provided good
"exercise" for Virgils new sight. Among the first objects he
had recognized, just the day after his bandages came off, were rolls of
toilet paper on display. He had picked up a package, and given it to Amy, to
prove he could see. Three days after surgery, they had
gone to an I.G.A., and Virgil had seen shelves, fruit, cans, people, aisles,
carts-so much that he got scared. "Everything ran together,"
he said. He needed to get out of the store and dose his eyes for a bit.
He enjoyed uncluttered views, he said, of green hills and grass - especially
after the over-full, over-rich visual spectacles of
shops - though it was difficult for him, Amy indicated, to connect the visual
shapes of hills with the tangible hills he had walked up;
and he had no idea of perspective. But the first month of seeing had been predominantly
positive: "Every day seems like a great
adventure, seeing more for the first time each day," Arny had written,
summarizing it, in her journal.
WHEN we arrived at the house, Virgil, caneless, walked by himself up the path
to the front door, pulled out his key, grasped the
doorknob, unlocked the door, and opened it. This was impressive7-he could never
have done it at first, he said--and was something he
had been practicing since the day after surgery. It was his showpiece. But he
said that in general he found walking "scary" and
"confusing" without touch, without his cane, with his uncertain, unstable
judgment of space and distance. Sometimes surfaces or
objects would seem to loom, to be on top of him, when they were still quite
a distance away; sometimes he would get confused by his
own shadow~the whole concept of shadows, of objects blocking light, was puzzling
to him-and would come to a stop, or trip, or try to
step over it. Steps, in particular, posed a'special hazard, because all he could
see was a confusion, a flat surface, of parallel and
crisscrossing lines; he could not see them (although he knew them) as solid
objects going up or cominig down in three-dimensional
space. Now, five weeks after surgery, he often felt more disabled than he had
felt when he was blind, and he had lost the confidence,
the ease of moving, that he had possessed then. But* he hoped all this would
sort itself out with time.
I was not so sure; every patient described in the literature had faced great
difficulties after surgery in the apprehension of
space and distanct--for months, even years. This was the case even in Valvo's
highly intelligent patient H.S., who had been normally
sighted until, at fifteen, he was corneally scarred by a chemical explosion,
and had regained his vision twenty-two years later. He
encountered grave difficulties of every kind, which he recorded, minutely, on
tape:
During these first weeks [after surgery] I had no a preciation of depth or distance;
street.lits were luminous stains stuck to the win
ow panes, and the corridors of the hospital were black holes. When I crossed
the road the traffic terrified me, even when I was
accompanied. I am very insecure while walking; indeed I am more afraid now than
before the operation.
WE gathered in the kitchen at the back of the house, which had a large white
deal table. Bob and I laid out all our test objects-color
charts, letter charts, pictures, illusions--on it, and we set up a video camera
to record the testing. As we settled down, Virgirs cat and
dog bounded in to greet and check usand Virgil, we noted, had some difficulty
telling which was which. This comic and embarrassing
problem had persisted since he returned home from surgery: both animals, as
it happened, were blackand-white, and he kept confusing
them-to their annoyance-unless he could touch them, too. Sometimes, Amy said,
she would see him examining the cat careffilly,
looking at its head, its ears, its paws, its tail, and touching each part gently
as he did so. I observed this myself the next day~Virgil
feeling and looking at Tibbles with extraordinary intentness, correlating the
cat. He would keep doing this, Amy remarked ("You'd
think once was enougl~% but the new ideas, the visual recognitions, kept slipping
from his mind.
Cheselden described a strikingly similar scene with his young patient in the
seventeen-twenties:
One particular only, though it might appear trifling, I will relate: Having
often forgot which was the cat, and which the dog, he was ashamed to
ask; but catching the cat, which he knew by feeling, he was observed to look
at her steadfastly, and then, setting her down, said, So, puss, I shall
know you another time .... Upon being told what things were ... he would carefully
observe that he might know them again; and (as he said) at
first learned to know, and again forgot, a thousand things in a day.
Virgil's first formal recognitions when the bandages were taken off had been
of letters on the ophthalmologist's eye chart, and
we decided to test him, first, on letter recognition. He could not see ordinary
newsprint clearly~his acuity was still only about
20/80-but he readily perceived letters that were more than a third of an inch
high. Here he did rather well, for the most part, and
recognized all the commoner letters (at least, capital letters) easily~as he
had been able to do from the moment the bandages were
removed. How was it that he had so much difficulty recognizing faces, or the
cat, and so much difficulty with shapes generally, and
with size and distance, and yet had little-or at least much less-difficulty
recognizing letters? When I asked Virgil about it, he told me
that he had learned the alphabet by touch at school, where they had used letter
blocks, or cutout letters, for teaching the blind. I was
much struck by this. Moreover, as Gregory relates of S.B., "much to our
surprise, he could even tell the time by means of a large clock
on the wall. We were so surprised at this that we did not at first believe that
he could have been in any sense blind before the
operation." But in his blind days S.B. had used a large hunter watch with
no glass, telling the time by touching the hands, and had
apparently made an instant "crossmodal" transfer, to use Gregorys
term, from touch to vision.
But while Virgil could recognize individual letters easily, he could not string
them together-could not read, or even see words.
I found this puzzling, for he said that they used not only Braille but English,
in raised or inscribed letters, at school-and that he had
teamed to read fairly fluently. 1ndeed, he could still easily read the inscriptions
on war memorials and tombstones by touch. But his
eyes seemed to fix on particular letters, and to be incapable of the easy movement,
the scanning, that is needed to read. This was also
the case with the literate H.S.:
My first attempts at reading were painful. I could make out single letters,
but it was impossible for me to make out whole words; I managed to do
so only after weeks of exhausting attempts. In fact, it was impossible for me
to remember all the letters together, after having read them one by
one. Nor was it possible for me, during the first weeks, to count my own five
fingers: I.had the feelin~ that they were all there, but ... it was not
possible for me to pass from one to the other wfiile counting.
Further problems became apparent as we spent the day with Virgil. He would pick
up details incessantly-an angle, an edge, a
color, a movement-but would not be able to synthesize them, to form a complex
perception at a glance. This was one reason the cat,
visually, was so puzzling: he would see a paw, the nose, the tail, an ear, but
could not see all of them together, see the cat as a whole.
Amy had commented in her journal on how even the most "obvious" connections-visually
and logically obvioushad to be
learned. Thus, Amy told us, a few days after the operation "he said that
trees didn't look like anything on earth," but in her entry for
October 21st, a month after the operation, she noted, "Virgil finally put
a tree together-he now knows that the trunk and leaves go
together to form a complete unit." And, on another occasion: "Skyscrapers
strange, cannot understand how they stay up without
collapsing."
Many - or perhaps all - patients in Virgil's situation had had similar difficulties.
One such patient (described by Eduard Raehlmann, in
1891), though she had had a little vision preoperatively and had frequently
handled dogs, "had no idea of how the head, legs, and ears
were connected to the animal." Of such difficulties, which may seem almost
incomprehensible, or absurd, to the rest of us, Valvo
remarks, "The real difficulty here is that simultaneous perception of objects
is an unaccustomed way to those used to sequential
perception through touch. We, with a fiffl complement of senses, live
in space and time; the blind live in a world of time alone. For
the blind build their worlds from sequences of impressions (tactile, auditory,
olfactory), and are not capable, as sighted people are, of a
simultaneous visual perception, the making of an instantaneous visual scene.
Indeed, if one can no longer see in space then the idea of
space becomes incomprehensibleand this even for highly intelligent people blinded
relatively late in life. This is powerffilly conveyed
by John Hull in his remarkable autobiography, "Touching the Rock,"
when he speaks of himself, of the blind, as "living in time"
almost exclusively. With the blind, he writes,
this sense of being in a place is less pronounced.... Space is reduced to one's
own body, and the position of the body is known not
by what objects have been passed.but ~y how long it has been in motion. Position
is thus measured by time.... For the blind,
people are not there unless they speak.... eople are in motion, they are temporal,
they come and they go. They come out of
nothing; they disappear.
ATHOUGH Virgil could recognize letters and numbers, and could write them, too,
he mixed up some rather similar ones ("A!' and "H,"
for example) and" on occasion, wrote some backward. (Hull describes how,
after only five years of blindness in his forties, his own
visual memories had become so uncertain that he was not sure which way around
a "Y' went, and had to trace it in the air with his
fingers.) Still, Virgil's performance was an impressive one for a man who had
not seen for forty-five years. But the world does not
consist of letters and numbers. How would he do with objects and pictures? How
would he do outside, in the real world?
His first impressions when the bandages were removed were especially of color,
and it seemed to be color, which has no
analogue in the world of touch, that excited and delighted him-this was very
clear from the way he spoke, and from Amys journal. (The
recognition of colors and movement seems to be innate.) It was colors to which
Virgil continually alluded, the chromatic
unexpectedness of new sights. He had had Greek salad and spaghetti the night
before, he told us, and the spaghetti startled him: "White
round strings, like fishing line," he said. "I thought itd be brown."
Seeing light and shape and movements, seeing colors above all, had been completely
unexpected, and had had a physical and
emotional impact almost shocking, explosive. ("I felt the violence of these
sensations," wrote Valvo's patient H.S., "like a blow on the
head. The violence of the emotion ... was akin to the very strong emotion I
felt on seeing my wife for the first time, and when out in a
car, I saw the huge monuments ofRome.")
We found that Virgil easily distinguished a great array of colors, and matched
them without difficulty. But, confusingly, or
confusedly, he sometimes gave colors the wrong names: yellow, for example, he
called pink, but he knew that it was the same color as
a banana. We wondered at first whether he could have a color agnosia or color
anomia-defects of color association and color naming
that are due to damage in specific areas of the brain. But these difficulties,
it seemed to us, came from lack of learning-frorn the fact
that early and long blindness had sometimes prevented his associating colors
with their names or had caused him to forget some of the
associations he had made. Such associations and the neural connections which
underlay them, feeble in the first place, had become
disestablished in his brain, not through any damage or disease but simply from
disuse.
Although Virgil believed that he had visual memories, including color memories,
from the remote past-on our drive from the
airport he had spoken of growing up on the farm in Kentucky ("I see the
creek running down the middle," "birds on the fences," "the
big old white house")-l could not decide whether these were genuine memories,
visual images in his mind, or mere verbal descriptions
without images (like Helen Kellcr's).
How was he with shapes? Here matters were more complicated, because in the weeks
since his surgery Virgil had been
practicing shapes, correlating their look and their feet. No such practice had
been required with colors. He had at first been unable to
recognize any shapes visually-even shapes as simple as a square or a circle,
which he recognized instantly by touch. To him, a touch
square in no sense corresponded to a sight square. This was his answer to the
Molyneux question. For this reason, Amy had bought,
among other things, a child's wooden pegboard, with large, simple blocks-square,
triangle, circle, and rectangle-to be fitted into
corresponding holes, and had got Virgil to practice with it every day. Virgil
found the task impossible at first, but quite easy now, after
practicing for a month. He still tended to feel the holes and shapes before
matching them, but when we forbade this he fitted them
together quite fluently by sight alone.
Solid objects, it was evident, presented much more difficulty, because their
appearance was so variable; and much of the past
five weeks had been devoted to the exploration of objects, their unexpected
vicissitudes of appearance as they were seen from near or
far, or half concealed, or from different places and angles.
On the day he returned home after the bandages were removed, his house and its
contents were unintelligible to him,, and he
had to be led up the garden path, led through the house, led into each room,
and introduced to each chair. Within a week, with Amys
help, he had established a canonical line-a particular line up the path, through
the sitting room to the kitchen, with further lines, as
necessary, to the bathroom and the bedroom. It was only from this line, at first,
that he could recognize anything-though this took a
great deal of interpretation and inference; thus, he learned, for example, that
"a whiteness to the right," to be seen as he came obliquely
through the front door, was in fact the dining table in the next room, although
at this point neither "table" nor "dining room" was a
clear visual concept. If he deviated from the line, he would be totally disoriented.
Then, carefully, with Amys help, he started to use the
line as a home base, making short sallies and excursions to either side of it,
so that he could see the room, feel its walls and contents
from different angles, and build up a sense of space, of solidity, of perspective.
As Virgil explored the rooms of his house, investigating, so to speal,, the
visual construction of the world, I was reminded of
an infant moving his hand to and fro before his c I yes, waggling his head,
turning it this way and that, in his primal construction of the
world. Most of us have no sense of the immensity of this construction, for we
perform it seamlessly, unconsciously, thousands of times
every day, at a glance. But this is not so for a baby, it was not so for Virgil,
and it is not so for, say, an artist who wants to experience
his perceptions individually and anew. C6zanne wrote to his son, "The same
subject seen from a different angle gives a subject for
study of the highest interest and so varied that I think I could be occupied
for months without changing my place, simply bending more
to the right or left."
We achieve perceptual constancy~ the correlation of all the different appearances,
the transforms of objects-very early, in the
first months of life. It constitutes a huge teaming task, yet is achieved so
swiftly and automatically that its enormous complexity is
scarcely realized (though it is an achievement that even the largest supercomputers
cannot begin to match). But for Virgil, with half a
century of forgetting whatever visual engrams he had constructed, the teaming,
or relearning, of these transforms required hours of
conscious and systematic exploration each day. This first month, then, saw a
systematic exploration, by sight and touch, of all the
smaller things in the house: ftuit, vegetables, bottles, cans, cutlery, flowers,
the knickknacks on the mantelpiece-turning them round
and round, holding them dose to him, then at arm's length, trying to synthesize
their varying appearances into a sense of unitary
objecthood.
Despite all the vexations that trying to see could entail, Virgil had stuck
with this gamely, and he had learned steadily. He had
little difficulty recognizing the fruit, the bottles, the cans in the kitchen,
the different flowers in the living room, and other common
objects in the house.
Unfamiliar objects were much more difficult. When I took a blood-pressure cuff
from my medical bag, he was completely
flummoxed and could make nothing of it, but he recognized it immediately when
I allowed him to touch it. Moving objects presented a
special problem, for their appearance changed constantly. Even his dog, he told
me, looked so different at different times that he
wondered if it was the same dog. He was utterly lost when it came to the rapid
changes in others' physiognomies. Such difficulties are
almost universal among the early blinded restored to sight. Gregorys patient
S.B. could not recognize individual faces, or their
expressions, a year after his eyes had been operated on, despite perfectly normal
elementary vision. What about pictures? Here I had
been given conflicting reports about Virgil. He was said to love television,
to follow everything on it-and, indeed, a huge new TV
stood in the living room, an emblem of Virgirs new life as a seeing person.
We tried him first on still pictures, pictures in magazines,
and with those he had no success at all. He could not see people, could not
see objects-did not comprehend the idea of representation.
It was similar with Cheselden's young patient:
We thought he soon knew what pictures represented ... but we found afterwards
we were mistaken; for about two months after he was couched,
he discovered at once they represented solid bodies, when to that time he considered
them only as party-coloured planes, or surfaces diversified
with variety of paint; but even then he was no less surprised, expecting the
pictures would feet like the things they represented, . . . and asked
which was the lying sense, feeling or seeing?
Nor were things any better with aioving pictures on a TV screen. Mind-fal of
Virgil's passion for listening to baseball games,
we found a channel with a game in progress. It seemed at first as if he were
following it visually, because he could describe who was
batting, what was going on. But as soon as we turned off the sound he was lost.
It became evident that he himself perceived little
beyond streaks of light and colors and motions, and that all the rest (what
he seemed to see) was interpretation, performed swiftly, and
perhaps unconsciously, in consonance with the sound. How it would be with a
real game we were far from sure-it seemed possible to
us that he might see and enjoy a good deal-but in the representation of reality,
pictorial or televisual, he was at this point completely at
sea.
Virgil had now had two hours of testing, and was beginning to get tiredboth
visually and cognitively tired, as he had tended to
do since the operationand when he got tired he could see less and less, and
had more and more difficulty making sense of what he
could see.
Indeed, we were getting restless ourselves, and wanted to get out after a morning
of testing. We asked him, as a final task
before going for a drive, if he felt up to some drawing. We suggested first
that he draw a hammer. (A hammer was the first object S.B.
drew.) Virgil agreed, and, rather shakily, began to draw. He tended to guide
the pencil's movement with his free hand. ("He only does
that because he's tired now," said Amy.) Then he drew a car (very high
and old-fashioned); a plane (with the tail missing: it would have
been hard put to fly); and a house (flat and crude, like a three-year-old's
drawing).
WHEN we finally got out, it was a brilliant October morning, and Virgil was
blinded for a minute, until he put on a pair of dark-green
sunglasses. Even ordinary daylight, he said, seemed far too bright for him,
too glary-, he felt that he saw best in quite subdued light.
We asked him where he would like to go, and after thinking for a little he said,
"The zoo." He had never been to a zoo, he said, and he
was curious to know how the different animals looked. He had loved animals ever
since his childhood days on the farm. ,
Very striking, as soon as we got to the zoo, was Virgil's sensitivity to motion.
He was startled, first, by an odd strutting
movement; it made him smile-he had -never seen anything like it. "What
is it?" he asked.
"An emu."
He wasn't quite sure what an emu was, so we asked him to describe it to us.
He had difficulty, and could say only that it was
about the same size as Amy~she and the emu were standing side by side at that
point-but that its movements were quite different from
hers. He wanted to touch it, to feel it all over. If he did that, he thought,
he would then see it better. But touching, sadly, was not
allowed.
His eye was caught next by a leaping motion nearby, and he immediately realized-or,
rather, surmised-that it must be a
kangaroo. His eye followed its motions closely, but he could not describe it,
he said, unless he could feel it. We were wondering by
now exactly what he could see7and what, indeed, he meant by "seeing."
In general, it seemed to us, if Virgil could identify an animal it would be
either by its motion or by virtue of a single
feature-thus, he might identify a kangaroo because it leaped, a giraffe by its
height, or a zebra by its stripes-but he could not form any
over-all impression of the animal. It was also necessary that the animal be
sharply defined against a background; he could not identify
the elephants, despite their trunks, because they were at a considerable distance
and stood against a slate-colored background.
Finally, we went to the great-ape en closure; Virgil was curious to see the
gorilla. He could not see it at all when it was half hidden
among some trees, and when it finally came into the open he thought that, though
it moved differently, it looked just like a large man.
Fortunately, there was a life-size bronze statue of a gorilla in the enclosure,
and we told Virgil, who had been longing to touch all the
animals, that he could, if nothing else, at least examine the statue. Exploring
it swiftly and minutely with his hands, he had an air of
assurance that he had never shown when examining anything by sight. It came
to me-perhaps it came to all of us at this moment-how
skillfiil and self-sufficient he had been as a blind man, how naturally and
easily he had experienced his world with his hands, and how
much we were now, so to speak, pushing him against the grain: demanding that
he renounce all that came easily to him, that he sense
the world in a way incredibly difficult for him, and alien.
His face seemed to light up with comprehension as he felt the statue. "It's
not like a man at all," he murmured. The statue
examined, he opened his eyes, and turned around to the real gorilla standing
before him in the enclosure. And now, in a way that would
have been impossible before, he described the ape's posture, the way the knuckles
touched the ground, the little bandy legs, the great
canines, the huge ridge on the head, pointing to each feature as he did so.
Gregory writes of a wonderful episode with his patient S.B.,
who had a long-standing interest in tools and machinery. Gregory took him to
the Science,Museum in London to see its grand
collection:
The most in ere . n : episode was his reaction to the ne eslay screw cutting
lathe which is housed in a special glass case.... We led
him to the glass case, which was closed, and asked him to tell us what was in
it. He was quite unable to say anything about it,
except that he thought the nearest part was a handle.... We then asked a museum
attendant (as previously arranged) for the case to
be opened, and S.B. was allowed to touch the lathe. The result was startling...
He ran his hands eagerly over the lathe ' with his
eyes tight shut. Then he stood back a little and opened his eyes and said: "Now
that I've felt it I can see."
So it was with Virgil and the gorilla. This spectacular example of how touching
could make seeing possible explained
something else that had puzzled me ' Since the operation, Virgil had begun to
buy toy soldiers, toy cars, toy animals, miniatures of
famous buildings-an entire Lilliputian world-and to spend hours with them. It
was not mere childishness or playfulness that had driven
him to such pastimes. Through touching these at the same time he looked at them,
he could forge a crucial correlation; he could
prepare himself to see the real world by learning first to see this toy world.
The disparity of scale did not matter, any more than it
mattered to S.B., who was instantly able to tell the time on a large wall clock
because he could correlate it with what he knew by touch
from his pocket watch.
For lunch, we repaired to a local fish restaurant, and as we ate I stole glances,
from time to time, at Virgil. He started eating, I
observed, in the normal sighted fashion, accurately spearing segments of tomato
in his salad. Then, as he contillued, his aim grew
worse: his fork started to miss its targets, and to hover,'uncertainly, in the
air. Finally, unable to "see," or make sense of, what was on
his plate, he gave up the effort and started to use his hands, to eat as he
used to, as a blind person eats. Amy had already told me about
such relapses, and described them in herjournal. There had been similar reversions,
for example, with his shaving, where he would start
with a mirror, shaving by sight, with tense concentration. Then the strokes
of the razor would become slower, and he would start to
peer uncertainly at his face in the mirror, or try to confirm what he half saw
by touch. Finally, he would turn away from the mirror, or
close his eyes, or turn the light off, and finish the job by feel.
That Virgil should have periods of acute visual fatigue following sustained
visual effort or use was scarcely surprising; A of
us have them if too much is demanded of our vision. Something happens to my
own visual system if, for instance, I look at EEGs
non-stop for three hours: I start missing things on the traces, and seeing dazzling
afterimages of the squiggles wherever I look-the wals,
the ceiling, all over the visual field--and at this point I need to stop and
do something else, or, even better, dose my eyes for an hour.
And Virgil's visual system, by comparison with the normal one, must have been
at this stage labile in the extreme.
Less easy to understand, and alarming, perhaps ominous, were long periods of
"blurriness"-impaired vision or gnosis-lasting
hours, or even days, coming on spontaneously, without obvious reason. Bob Wasserman
was very much puzzled by Virgils and Amy's
descriptions of these fluctuations; he had been practicing ophthalmology for
some twenty-five years, and removed many cataracts, but
he had never encountered fluctuations of this sort.
After lunch, we all went to Dr. Hamlin's office. Dr. Hamlin had taken detailed
photographs of the retina right after surgery,
and Bob, examining the eye now (with both direct and indirect ophthalmoscopy)
and comparing it with the photographs, could see no
evidence of any postoperative complications. (A special test-fluorescein angiography~
had shown a small degree of cystoid macular
edema, but this would not have caused the rapid fluctuations that were so striking.)
Because there seemed to be no adequate local or
ocular cause for these fluctuations, Bob wondered whether they could be a consequence
of some underlying medical condition-we had
been struck by how unwell Virgil looked as soon as we met him--or whether they
could represent a neural reaction of the brain's visual
system to conditions of sensory or cognitive overload. It is no effort for the
normally sighted to construct shapes, boundaries, objects,
and scenes from purely visual sensations; they have been making such visual
constructs, a visual world, from the moment of birth, and
have developed a vast, effortless cognitive apparatus for doing so. (Ordinarily,
a half of the cerebral cortex is given over to visual
processing.) But in Virgil these cognitive powers, undeveloped, were rudimentary;
the visualcognitive parts of his brain might easily
have been overwhelmed.
Brain systems in all animals may respond to overwhelming stimulation, or stimulation
past a critical point, with a sudden
shutdown. Such reactions have nothing to do with the individual or his motives.
They are purely local and physiological, and can occur
even in isolated slices of cerebral cortex: they are a biological defense against
neural overload.
Still, perceptual-cognitive processes, while physiological, are also personal-it
is not a world which one perceives or constructs
but one's own world-and they lead to, are linked to, a perceptual self, with
a will, an orientation, and a style of its own. This perceptual
self may also collapse with the collapse of perceptual systems, and alter the
orientation and the very identity of the individual. If this
occurs, an individual not only becomes blind but ceases to behave as a visual
being, yet offers no report of any change in inner state, is
completely oblivious of his own visuality or lack of it. Such a condition, of
total psychic blindness (known as Anton's syndrome), may
occur if there is massive damage, as from a stroke, to the visual parts of the
brain. But it also seemed to occur, on occasion, with Virgil.
At such times, indeed, he might talk of "seeing" while in fact appearing
blind and showing no visual behavior whatever. One had to
wonder whether the whole basis of visual perception and identity in Virgil was
as yet so feeble that under conditions of overload or
exhaustion he might go in and out of not merely physical blindness but a total
Anton-like psychic blindness.
A QUITE different sort of visual shutdown-a withdrawal-seems to be associated
with situations of great emotional stress or conflict.
And for Virgil this period was indeed as stressful a time as he had ever known:
he had just had surgery, he had just been married; the
even tenor of his blind, bachelor life had been shattered; he was under a tremendous
pressure of expectation; and seeing itself was
confusing, exhausting. These pressures had increased as his wedding day approached,
especially with the convergence of his own
family in town; his family had not only opposed the surgery in the first place
but now insisted that he was in fact still blind. All this was
documented by Amy in herjournal:
October 9: Went to church to decorate for wedding. Virgil's vision quite blurry.
Not able to distinguish much. It is as though
sight has taken a nosedive. Virgill acting "blind" again... Having
me lead him around.
October 11: Virgil's family arrives today. His sight seems to have gone on vacation....
It is as though he has gone back to being
blind! FamiLy arrived. Couldn't believe he could see. Every time he said he
could see something they would say, "Ah, you're just
guessing." They treated him as though he was totally blind-leading him
around, giving im an bing he wanted....I am very nervous,
anYVirgil's sight has disageared.... Want to be sure we are doing t e right
thing.
October 12: Wedding day. Virgil very calm ... vision little clearer, but still
blurry.... Could see me coming down aisle, but was
very blurry.... Wedding beautiful. Party at Mom's. Virgil surrounded by family.
They still cannot accept his sight, he could not
see much. Said goodbye to his family tonight. Sight began clearing up right
after they left.
Thus there seemed to be two distinct forms of "blind behavior" or
"acting blind"-one a collapse of visual identity on a
neurological basis, the other a massive withdrawal or regression on a strategic
basis, in the face of overwhelming emotional stress.
Then again, given the extreme physiological instability of his vision, who was
to know exactly what was going on? When a
specific organic weakness exists, emotional stress can easily press toward a
physical (or psychosomatic) form; thus, asthmatics get
asthma under stress, parkinsonians become more parkinsonian, and someone like
Virgil, with borderline vision, may get pushed over
the border and become (temporarily) blind. It was, therefore, exceedingly difficult
at times to distinguish between what was
physiological and what was psychological in him-what was "motivated behavior"
and what was physiological weakness.
Marius von Senden, reviewing every published case over a three-hundred-year
period in his classic book "Space and Sight"
(1932), concluded that every newly sighted adult sooner or later comes to a
"motivation crisis"-and that not every patient gets through
it. He tells of one patient who felt so threatened by sight (which would have
meant his leaving the Asylum for the Blind, and his
fianc6e there) that he threatened to tear his eyes out; he cites case after
case of patients who "behave blind" or "refuse to see" after
an
operation, and of others who, fearful of what sight may entail, refuse operation
(one such account, entitled "L'Aveugle Qui Refiise de
Voir," was published as early as 1771). Both Gregory and Valvo dilate on
the emotional dangers of forcing a new sense on a blind
man-how, after an initial exhilaration, a devastating (and even lethal) depression
could ensue.
Precisely such a depression descended on Gregory's patient: S.B.'s period in
the hospital was fiill of excitement and
perceptual progress. But the promise was not fiilfilled. Six months after the
opera-, tion, Gregory reports,
we formed a strong impression that his sight was to him almost entirely disappointing.
It enabled him to do a little more ... but it
became clear that the opportunities it afforded him were less than he had imagined....
He still to a great extent lived the rife of a
blind man, sometimes not bothering to put on the Zli ht at ni ht He did not
get on well with his neig s [now], who regarded him
as 11 odd," and his workmates [previously so admiring] played tricks on
him and teased him for being unable to read.
S.B.'s depression deepened, he became iU, and, two years after his operation,
he died. He had been perfectly healthy, he had once
enjoyed life; he was only fifty-four.
Valvo provides us with six exemplary tales, and a profound discussion, of the
feelings and behavior of early-blinded people
when they are confronted with the "gift" of sight, and with the necessity
of renouncing one world, one identity, for another.
A major conflict in Virgil, as in all newly sighted people, was the uneasy relation
of touch and sight-not knowing whether to
look or feel. This was obvious in Virgil from the day of the operation, and
was very evident the day we saw him, when he could hardly
keep his hands off the pegboard, longed to touch all the animals, and gave up
spearing his food. His vocabulary, his whole sensibility,
his picture of the world, was couched in tactile-or, at least, nonvisual-terms.
He was, or had been until his operation, a touch person
through and through.
It has been well established that in congenitally deaf people (especially if
they are native signers) some of the auditory parts of
the brain are reallocated for visual use. It has also been well established
that in blind people who read Braille the reading finger has an
exceptionally large representation in the tactile parts of the cerebral cortex.
And one would suspect that the tactile (and auditory) parts
of the cortex are enlarged in the blind, and may even extend into what is normally
visual cortex. What remains of the visual cortex,
without visual stimulation, may be largely undeveloped. It seems likely that
such a differentiation of cerebral development would
follow the early loss of a sense and the compensatory enhancement of other senses.
If this was the case in Virgil, what might happen if visual function was suddenly
made possible, demanded? One might
certainly expect some visual learning, some development of new pathways in the
visual parts of the brain. There had never been any
documentation of the kindling of activity in the visual cortex of an adult,
and we hoped to take special PET scans of Virgil's visual
cortex to show this as he learned to see. But what would this learning, this
activation, be like? Would it be like a baby first learning to
see? (This was Amys first thought.) But the newly sighted are not on the same
starting line, neurologically speaking, as babies, whose
cerebral cortex is "equipotential"-equally ready to adapt to any form
of perception. The cortex of an early-blinded adult such as Virgil
has already become highly adapted to organizing perceptions in time and not
in space.
An infant merely learns. This is a huge, never-ending task, but it is not one
charged with irresoluble conflict. A newly sighted
adult, bycontrast, has to make a radical switch from a sequential to a visualspatial
mode, and such a switch flies in the face of the
experience of an entire lifetime. Gregory emphasizes this, pointing out how
conflict and crisis are inevitable if "the perceptual habits
and strategies of a lifetime" are to be changed. Such conflicts are built
into the nature of the nervous system itself, for the early~blinded
~fdult who has spent a lifetime adapting and specializing his brain must now
ask his brain to reverse all this. (Moreover, the brain of an
adult no longer has the plasticity of a child's brain7-that is why learning
new languages or new skills becomes more difficult with age.
But in the case of a man previously blind, learning to see is not like learning
another language; it is, as Diderot puts it, like learning
language for the first time.)
In the newly sighted, learning to see demands a radical change in neurological
functioning and, with it, a radical change in
psychological functioning, in self, in identity. The change may be experienced
in literally life-and-death terms. Valvo quotes a patient
of his as saying, "One must die as a sighted person to be bom again as
a blind person," and the opposite is equally true: one must die as
a blind person to be born again as a seeing person. And here, blindness is no
more a negative condition, a privation, than seeing. It is a
d!yerent condition, a different form of being, with its own sensibilities and
coherence and feeling. It is indeed what John Hull calls
"deep blindness ... one of the orders of human being."
0N October 31st, the cataract in Virgirs left eye was removed, and revealed
a retina, an acuity, similar to the right. This.was a great
disappointment, for there had been hope that it might be a far better eye-enough
to make a crucial difference to his vision. His vision
did improve slightly: he fixated better, and the searching eye movements were
fewer, and he had a larger visual field.
With both eyes working, Virgil now went back to work, but found, increasingly,
that there was another side to seeing, that much of it
was confusing, and some downright shocking. He had worked happily at the Y for
thirty years, he said, and thought he knew all the
bodies of his clients. Now he found himself startled by seeing bodies, and skins,
that he had previously known only by touch; he was
amazed at the range of skin colors he saw, and slightly disgusted by blemishes
and "stains" in skins that to his hands had seemed
perfectly smooth. (Gregory observes of S.B., "He also found some things
he loved ugly (including his wife and himself), and he was
frequently upset by the blemishes and imperfections of the visible world.")
Virgil found it a relief, when giving massages, to shut his
eyes.
He continued to improve, visually, over the ensuing weeks, especially when he
was free to set his own pace. He did his utmost
to live the life of a sighted man, but he also became more conflicted at this
time. He expressed fears, occasionally, that he would have
to throw away his cane and walk outside, cross the streets, by vision alone;
and, on one occasion, a fear that he might be "expected" to
drive, and take up an entirely new, sighted job. This, then, was a time of great
striving and real success-but success achieved, one felt,
at a psychological cost, at a cost of deepening strain and splitting in himself.
There had been one outing, a week before Christmas, when he and Amy went to
the ballet. Virgil enjoyed "The Nutcracker":
he had always loved the music, and now, for the first time, he saw something
as well. "I could see people jumping around the stage.
Couldn't see what they were wearing, though," he said. He thought he would
enjoy seeing a live baseball game, and looked forward to
the start of the season in the spring.
Christmas was a particularly festive and important time-the first Christmas
after his wedding, his first Christmas as a sighted
man-and he returned, with Amy, to the family farm in Kentucky. He saw his mother
for the first time in more than forty years-he had
scarcely been able to see her, to see anything much, at the time of the wedding-and
thought she looked "real pretty." He saw again the
old farmhouse, the fences, the creek in the pasture, which he had also not seen
since he was a child; he had never ceased to cherish
them in his mind. Some of his seeing had been a great disappointment, but seeing
home and family was not-it was a pure joy.
No less important was the change in the familys attitude toward him. "He
seemed more alert," his sister said. "He would walk,
move around the house, without touching the walls-he would just get up and go."
She felt that there had been "a big difference" since
he was first operated on, and his mother and the rest of the family felt the
same.
I phoned them, the day before Christmas, and spoke to his mother, his sister,
and others. They asked me to join them, and I
wish I could have done so, for it seemed to be an affirmative time for them
all. The familys initial opposition to Virgil's seeing (and
perhaps to Amy, too, for having "pushed" it) and their disbelief that
he could actually see had been something that he internalized,
something that could literally annihilate his seeing. Now that the family was
"converted," a major psychological block, one hoped,
might dissolve. Christmas was the climax, but also the resolution, of an extraordinary
year.
What would happen, I wondered, in the coming year? What might he hope for, at
best? How much of a visual world, a visual
life, might still await him? We were, frankly, quite unsure at this point. Grim
and frightening though the histories of so many patients
were, some, at least, overcame the worst of their difficulties and emerged into
a relatively unconflicted new sight.
Valvo, normally cautious in expression, lets himself go a little in describing
some of his patients'happier outcomes:
Once our patients acquire visual patterns, and can work with them autonomously,
they seem to experience great joy in visual
learning ... a renaissance of personality.... They start thinking about wholly
new areas of experience.
A renaissance of personality-this was just what Amy wanted for Virgil. It was
difficult for us to imagine such a "renaissance"
in him, for he seemed so phlegmatic, so set in his ways. Despite a range of
problems-retinal, cortical, psychological, possibly
medical-he had done remarkably well in a way, had shown a steady ability to
increase his power to apprehend a visual world. There
seemed no reason that, with his predominantly positive motivation, and the obvious
enjoyment and advantage he could get from
seeing, he should not progress further. He could never hope to have perfect
vision, but he certainly might hope for a life radically
enlarged by seeing.
THE catastrophe, when it came, was very sudden. On February 8th, I had a phone
call from Amy: Virgil had collapsed, had been taken,
gray and stuporous, to the hospital. He had a lobar pneumonia, a massive consolidation
of one lung, and was in the intensive-care unit,
on oxygen and intravenous antibiotics.
The first antibiotics used did not work: he grew worse; he grew critical; and
for some days he hovered between life and death.
Then, after three weeks, the infection was finally mastered, and the lung started
to re-expand. But Virgil himself remained gravely ill,
for, though the pneumonia itself was clearing, it had tipped him into respiratory
failure - a non-paraJysis of the respiratory center in the
brain, which made it unable to respond properly to levels of oxygen and carbon
dioxide in the blood. The oxygen levels in his blood
started to fall - fell to less than half of normal. And the level of carbon
dioxide started to rise - rose to nearly three times normal. He
needed oxygen constantly, but only a little could be given, lest his failing
respiratory center be further depressed. With his brain
deprived of oxygen and poisoned' by carbon dioxide, Virgils consciousness
fluctuated and faded, and on bad days (when the oxygen
in his blood was lowest and the carbon dioxide highest) he could see nothing:
he was totally blind.
Much contributed to this continuing respiratory crisis: Virgil's lungs themselves
were thickened and fibrotic; there was
advanced bronchitis and emphysema; there was no movement of the diaphragm on
one side, a consequence of his childhood polio;
and, on top of all this, he was enormously obese-obese enough to cause a Pickwickian
syndrome (named after the somnolent fat boy,
Joe, in "The Pickwick Papers"). In Pickwickian syndrome, there is
a grave depression of breathing, and failure to oxygenate the blood
fully, associated with a depression of the respiratory center in the brain.
Virgil had probably been getting ill for some years; he had gradually been increasing
in weight since 1985. But between his
wedding and Christmas he had put on forty pounds-had shot up, in a few weeks,
to two hundred and eighty pounds-partly from fluid
retention caused by heart failure, and partly from non-stop eating, a habit
of his under stress.
So he now had to spend three weeks in the hospital, his blood oxygen still plummeting
to dangerously low levels, despite his
being given oxygen-and each time the level grew really low he became lethargic
and totally blind. Amy would know the moment she
opened his door what sort of day he was having where the blood oxygen was-depending
on whether he used his eyes, looked around, or
fumbled and touched, acted blind. (We wondered, in retrospect, whether the strange
fluctuations his vision had shown from almost the
day of surgery might be explained, at least in part, by fluctuations in his
blood oxygen, with consequent retinal or cerebral anoxia.
Virgil could have been close to respiratory failure and anoxia even before his
acute illness.)
There was another, intermediate state, which Amy found very puzzling; at such
times, he would say that he saw nothing
whatever, but would reach for objects, avoid obstacles, and behave as if seeing.
Amy could make nothing of this singular state, in which
he manifestly responded to objects, could locate them, was seeing, and yet denied
any consciousness of it. This condition, called
implicit sight, unconscious sight, or blindsight, occurs if the visual parts
of the cerebral cortex are knocked out (as they might be by a
lack of oxygen, for instance) but the visual centers in the subcortex remain
intact. Visual signals are perceived and are responded to
appropriately, but nothing of this perception reaches consciousness at all.
At last, Virgil was able to leave the hospital and return home, but to return
a respiratory cripple. He was tethered to an oxygen
cylinder, and could not even stir from his chair without it. It seemed unlikely
at this stage that he would ever recover sufficiently to go
out and work again, and the Y now felt that it had to terminate his job. A few
months later, he was forced to leave the house where he
had lived as an employee of the Y. This was the situation last summer: Virgil
had lost not only his health but his job and his house as
well.
BY October, however, he was feeling better, and was able to go without oxygen
for an hour or two at a time. It had not been wholly
clear to me, from speaking to Virgil and Amy, what had finally happened to his
vision after all these months. Amy said that it had
"almost gone" but that now she felt it was coming back as he got better.
When I phoned the visual-rehabilitation center where Virgil
had been evaluated, I was given a different story. Virgil, I was told, seemed
to have lost all the sight restored the previous year, with
only a few bits remaining. Kathy, his therapist, thought he saw colors but little
else-and sometimes colors without objects: thus he
might see a haze or halo of pink around a PeptoBismol bottle without clearly
seeing the bottle itself. This color perception, she said,
was the only seeing that was constant; for the rest he appeared almost blind,
missed objects, groped, seemed visually lost. He was
showing his old, blind random movements of the eyes. And yet sometimes, spontaneously,
out of the blue, he would get sudden,
startling moments of vision, in which he would see objects, quite small ones.
But these percepts would then "vanish," as suddenly as
they came, and he was usually unable to retrieve them. For all practical purposes,
she said, Virgil was now blind.
I was shocked and puzzled when Kathy told me this. These were phenomena radically
different from anything he had shown
before: what was happening now with his eyes and his brain? From a distance,
I could not sort out what was happening, especially
since Amy, for her part, maintained that Virgil's vision was now improving.
Indeed, she got furious when she heard anyone say that
Virgil was blind, and she maintained that the visual-rehab center was actually
"teaching him to be blind." So in February of 1993, a
year after the onset of his devastating illness, we brought Virgil and Amy to
New York, to see us again and to get some specialized
physiological tests of retinal and brain function.
AS soon as I met Virgil, at the arrival gate at LaGuardia Airport, I could see
for myself that everything had gone quite terribly wrong.
He was now almost fifty pounds heavier than when I had met him in Oklahoma.
He was carrying a cylinder of oxygen strung over one
shoulder. He groped, his eyes wandered, he looked totally blind. Amy guided
him, her hand under his elbow, everywhere they went.
And yet sometimes as we drove over the Fifty-ninth Street Bridge into the city
he would pick up something-a light on the bridge - not
guessing but seeing it quite accurately. But he could never hold it or retrieve
it, and so remained visually lost.
When we came to test him in my office - first using large colored targets, then
large movements and flashlights - he missed
everything. He seemed totally blind - blinder than be bad been before his operations,
because then, at least, even through his cataracts
he could detect light, its direction, and the shadow of a hand moving before
him. Now he could detect nothing whatever, no longer
seemed to have any light-sensitive receptors: it was as if his retinas had gone.
Yet not totally gone - that was the odd thing. For once in
a while he would see something accurately: once, he saw, described, grasped
a banana; on two occasions, he was able to follow a
randomly moving light bar with his hands on a computer screen; and sometimes
he would reach for objects, or "guess" them correctly,
even though he said he saw "nothing" at such times-the blindsight
that had first been observed in the hospital.
We were dismayed at his nearuniform failure, and he was sinking into a demoralized,
defeated state - it was time to stop
testing and take a break for lunch. As we passed him a bowl of fruit, and he
felt the fruit with swift, sensitive, skillful fingers, his face
lighted up, and he regained his animation. He gave us, as he handled the fruit,
remarkable tactile descriptions, speaking of the waxy,
slick quality of the plum skin, the soft fuzz of peaches and smoothness of nectarines
("like a babys cheeks"), and the rough, dimpled
skin of oranges. He weighed the fruits in his hand, spoke of their weight and
consistency, their pips and stones; and then, lifting them to
his nose, their different smells. We included an exceedingly clever wax pear
among the real fruit; with its realistic shape and coloring,
it had deceived sighted people completely. Virgil was not taken in for a moment:
he burst out laughing as soon as he touched it. "It's a
candle," he said immediately, somewhat puzzled. "Shaped like a bell
or a pear." While he may indeed have been, in von Senden's
words, an exile from spatial reality," he was deeply at home in the
world of touch in time.
But if his sense of touch was perfectly preserved, there were now, it was evident,
just sparks from his retinas-rare, momentary
sparks, from retinas that now seemed to be ninety-nine per cent dead. Bob Wasserman,
too, who had not seen Virgil since our visit to
Oklahoma, was appalled at the degradation of vision, and wanted to reexamine
the retinas. When he did so, they looked exactly as
before - piebald, with areas of increased and decreased pigmentation. There
was no evidence of any new disease. Yet the functioning
of even the preserved areas of retina had fallen to almost zero. Electroretinograms,
designed to record the retina's electrical activity
when stimulated by light, were negative, and visual evoked potentials, designed
to show activity in the visual parts of the brain, were
absent-there was nothing, electrically, going on in either the retinas or the
brain that could be recorded. This could not be attributed to
the original disease, retinitis, which had long been inactive. Something else
had emerged in the past year and had, in effect,
extinguished his remaining retinal function.
We remembered how Virgil had constantly complained of glare, even on relatively
dull, overcast days-how glare seemed to
blind him sometimes, so that he needed the darkest glasses. Was it possible
(as my colleagues Kevin Halligan and Gerald Edelman
suggested) that with the removal of his cataractscataracts that had perhaps
shielded his fragile retinas for decades-the ordinary light of
day had proved lethal, burned his retinas? It is said that patients with other
retinal problems, like macular degeneration, may be
exceedingly intolerant of light-not merely ultraviolet but light of all wavelengths-and
that light may hasten the degeneration of their
retinas. Was this what had happened with Virgil? It was one possibility-the
Halligan-Edelman hypothesis. Should we have foreseen it,
and rationed Virgil's sight, or the ambient light, in some way?
Another possibility - a likelier one - related to Virgil's continuing hypoxia,
the fact that he had not had properly oxygenated
blood for a year. We had clear accounts of his vision waxing and waning in the
hospital as his blood gases went up and down. Could
the repeated, or continuing, oxygen-starving of his retinas (and perhaps also
of the visual areas of his cortex) have been the factor that
did them in? The extinction, in either case, seems to be irreversible.
THIS, then, is Virgil's story, the story of a "miraculous" restoration
of sight to a blind man, a story basically similar to that of
Cheselden's young patient in 1728, and of a handful of others over the past
three centuries-but with a bizarre and ironic twist at the end.
Gregory's patient, so well adapted to blindness before his operation, was first
delighted with seeing, but soon encountered intolerable
stresses and difficulties, found the "gift" transformed to a curse,
became deeply depressed, and soon after died. Almost all the earlier
patients, indeed, after their initial euphoria, were overwhelmed by the enormous
difficulties of adapting to a new sense; though a very
few, as Valvo stresses, have adapted and done well. Could Virgil have surmounted
these difficulties and adapted to seeing where so
many others had foundered on the way?
We shall never know, for the business of adaptation-and, indeed, of life as
he knew it-was suddenly cut across by a gratuitous
blow of fate: an illness that, at a single stroke, deprived him of job, house,
health, and independence, leaving him a gravely sick man,
unable to fend for himself. For Amy, who incited the surgery in the first place,
and who was so passionately invested in Virgil's seeing,
it was a "miracle" that misfired, a calamity. Virgil, for his part,
maintains philosophically, "These things happen." But he has been
shattered by this blow, has given vent to outbursts of rage: rage at his helplessness
and sickness; rage at the smashing of a promise and
a dream; and, beneath this, most fundamental of all, a rage that had been smoldering
in him almost from the beginning-rage at being
thrust into a battle he could neither renounce nor win. At the beginning, there
was certainly amazement, wonder, and sometimes joy.
There was also, of course, great courage. It was an adventure, an excursion
into a new world, the like of which is given to few. But
then came the problems, the conflicts, of seeing but not seeing, not being able
to make a visual world, and at the same time being
forced to give up his native competence-a torment from which no escape seemed
possible. But then, paradoxically, a release was given,
in the form of a second and now final blindness-a blindness he received as a
gift. Now, at last, Virgil is allowed to not see, allowed to
escape from the glaring, confusing world of sight and space, and to return to
his own true being, the touch world that has been his
home for almost fifty years.